Retrieval-Based Learning Strategies in Medical Education

Retrieval-Based Learning Strategies in Medical Education

November 22, 2021

  ·  

Satria Nur Sya’ban, MD, Adonis Wazir, MD, Peter Horneffer, MD

 | 

 | 

There are several evidence-based, data-driven approaches to education that promote effective long-term knowledge retention, the most important of which are retrieval-based learning strategies. Retrieval is not only an important evaluative tool in knowledge testing but also one of the most practical ways to make learning durable. The effectiveness of retrieval-based strategies has been convincingly demonstrated in numerous cognitive science studies, and the neurophysiological basis of this effectiveness is also becoming better understood. This article will review the evidence that supports the use of retrieval-based strategies and will provide recommendations for their implementation.

TABLE OF CONTENTS

Introduction

The ever-expanding body of medical knowledge makes it increasingly crucial to help medical students make use of the most beneficial learning techniques. Research in the field of cognitive science has demonstrated that certain strategies can be highly effective yet are often underutilized. Retrieval of information from long-term memory upon demand(1-3) is a potent memory enhancement technique.(4) Recent work has proven that retrieval is critical for “robust, durable, and long-term learning”,(5) and this applies not only to studying but also to learning in the classroom. Moreover, retrieval helps to “create coherent and integrated mental representations of complex concepts, the kind of deep learning necessary to solve new problems and draw new inferences.”(5) This is a goal for all educators, but in particular medical educators, whose task is to instill durable learning in the minds of future practitioners.

This article will focus on retrieval-based techniques, which include spaced retrieval, active recall, and the testing effect, as well as the evidence supporting the employment of retrieval strategies in learning. Research has shown that these tools can support the goal of fostering durable learning in our future medical providers. Furthermore, these strategies can be implemented with relative ease and effectiveness using technological solutions.

The educational tools of spaced retrieval, active recall, and the testing effect are interrelated and are sometimes referred to as active retrieval, spaced learning, distributed practice, spaced repetition, and the spacing effect. They are all examples of methods that produce desirable difficulties, mental efforts that are rewarded by enhanced memory.(6) Just as athletes come to expect short-term challenges in order to reap the benefits of strength and endurance, the brain may function at its best when it is also taxed appropriately, and the reward is abiding knowledge retention—something that is necessary for an excellent medical provider.(7) As Ebbinghaus depicted in his famous forgetting curve, there is a predictable pattern to fading memory. The goal of practicing retrieval is to disrupt that forgetting pattern (see the image above). Retrieval is best implemented just as forgetting occurs, and that is why the learner experiences hardship.(8)

What are Retrieval-Based Learning Strategies? 

A short review of the educational methods of spaced learning and interleaving will help explain retrieval-based strategies.(9,10) Spaced learning involves spreading out sessions of teaching or studying focused on a specific topic or concept over increasingly longer periods with interspersed breaks (see the figure below).(11,12) In utilizing spaced learning, students should strive to increase the interstudy time for content with which they are more familiar and decrease the interstudy time for material that is still being learned, as illustrated in the figure below. (13) In the strategy of interleaving, related concepts are mixed up in a particular learning or study session to maximize durable learning. There are elements of spaced practice and interleaving in retrieval strategies in that what is retrieved can be distributed over time or mixed in with other related concepts.

The Types of Spaced Retrieval. Differentiated by the length of interstudy intervals, the two main types of spaced retrieval are equal-interval and expanding-interval spaced retrieval.(13)

Equal expanding learning schedule

The primary goal of retrieval-based learning strategies is to learn the material such that the knowledge gained endures, rather than only to perform well on an assessment.(14) The benefits of effortful retrieval are the consolidation of what has been learned and the increased retrievability of this knowledge at a later time.(14) Furthermore, this retrieval action improves retention as well as promotes the transfer of knowledge to what is already known by the learner.(15) The three related retrieval-based strategies that are the focus of this article are more than the effortful recovery of information from a learner’s brain; they are powerful learning strategies in and of themselves.

Spaced retrieval is a type of spaced or distributed learning, sharing qualities of both spaced learning and retrieval practice.(16) It is the recalling of knowledge on a certain topic over varying periods of time. This interstudy interval must be just right: too short and the information recalled is still in working memory and not in long-term memory; too long and the retrieval might not be successful—frustration that is too great will be at odds with the motivation needed to continue effortful learning. Although there is still debate about the most beneficial pattern for a spaced retrieval protocol,(17–19) there is ample agreement that spaced retrieval produces better long-term retention than other studying methods and is “a key process for understanding learning and promoting learning.”(5)

Active recall is a type of active studying, requiring the retrieval of requested information in an arduous way (as opposed to the learner rereading a passage in a textbook, for example). Active studying techniques involve working with the material in an effortful way that facilitates retention. In active retrieval, a learner can be asked to remember information by either free recall, an open-ended question with no parameters, or guided or cued recall, for which the learner is given some kind of structured prompt to guide the course of the answer.(12)

The testing effect refers to the outcome of enhanced memory as a result of taking a test and shares some characteristics of both spaced retrieval and active recall.(20) Although testing is considered to be primarily an assessment tool, studies have long shown that the action of taking low-stakes practice tests leads to an increase in retention compared with spending an equal amount of time studying.(21) In this sense, testing in itself is a type of retrieval.

Evidence for Retrieval-Based Learning Strategies

Retrieval needs to be effortful to be impactful, but the retrieval task must be in line with the level of understanding that is attainable for the learner. The level of desirability varies according to the level of knowledge of the learner: When learning occurs without sufficient knowledge and background skill to solve the presented problem, the difficulty becomes undesirable and no longer promotes learning.(22) This understanding is foundational for retrieval strategies to be effective.

Some impressive results from research on the effectiveness of the repeated use of spaced retrieval compared with single recall or repeated mass retrieval (repeated retrieval with no spacing) can be seen in the figure below.(23) This research illustrates the powerful effects that repeated spaced retrieval has on retention, and another related study showed retrieval’s superiority over studying with a concept map.(24)

Performance on Final Recall, Comparing Different Study Strategies. Repeated spaced retrieval is superior to other study strategies for improving final recall.(23)

Study strategies

Further evidence for retrieval can be seen in a study that compared simply bringing to mind information (covert retrieval) to overtly retrieving information (such as answering a question through speech or writing). The study found that both retrieval methods outperformed restudying the material on a final assessment but that covert retrieval produced a higher performance on free recall tests (see figure below).(25) This study suggests that it is important to implement retrieval activities in learning regardless of the specific type of retrieval strategy.

Performance on Final Recall, Comparing Overt and Covert Retrieval Methods. Both overt and covert retrieval improve final recall over restudying techniques.(25)

004 performance on final free recall test

Some research on effective learning techniques has looked at spaced learning and retrieval practices. Research focusing on the effectiveness of 10 learning techniques found that spaced learning and practice testing were two of the most highly effective learning strategies across different learner ages and abilities, boosting student performance across many criterion tasks, and that practice testing offered particular ease of use.(26) A recent meta-analysis showed that just taking a practice test before a final test on the same material produced substantial gains in both learning and retention compared with all other learning conditions considered in the research.(27)

There are several studies that have focused on the effects of retrieval in premedical and medical students. In a study involving anatomy and physiology students, two experimental groups were given practice quizzes on previously learned material, one employing expanding-interval retrieval and the other equal-interval retrieval. The study found that, although there was no significant difference between the two spaced practice categories, both experimental groups had a 41% better retention of information compared with the control group, which did not engage in retrieval practice.(28) In line with the outcomes of this research, a study involving medical students taking a course on in-hospital resuscitation found that the experimental group, who received 3.5 hours of instruction followed by 0.5 hours of testing, had a mean score of 82.8% on assessment compared with the control group, who received only instruction and training but no testing and scored an average of 73.3%.(29) Other studies involving medical students found that they showed improved retention of clinical knowledge and better performance on standardized tests when they employed self-guided spaced practice or spaced retrieval problem sets.(30,31)

Why Retrieval Works

Insights from cognitive science

Cognitive scientists have gained insights into why retrieval works. In their book Make It Stick, Brown et al. summarize the research on retrieval-based strategies and conclude: “When retrieval practice is spaced, allowing some forgetting to occur between tests, it leads to stronger long-term retention than when it is massed.”(8) Why is retrieval, and specifically spaced retrieval, so powerful? The authors explain that passive study tactics (like rereading) create illusions of knowing that can be misleading to the learner regarding his or her true knowledge competency, whereas retrieval strategies hinder forgetting because practice fosters a strengthening of that information in long-term memory (5,14) and helps the learner retrieve it more easily the next time.(8) This retrieval-mediated learning benefit is thought to be due to the “co-activation of related (semantic) information during retrieval,” strengthening the associated neural network. It also appears that retrieval “supports the creation of a generalized memory trace.”(4)

There is a positive metacognitive benefit for both educators and students in that taking quizzes or tests gives feedback as to how well learning is advancing.(32) Spaced retrieval “produces knowledge that can be retrieved flexibly and transferred to other situations.” When students use self-testing with corrected feedback, they can thus assess their own knowledge gaps and become more self-sufficient learners.(32) A review of learning efficiency in medical education concluded that retrieval practice using short-answer tests had an advantage over multiple-choice tests but that feedback was required for it to be beneficial.(33) Another study concluded that learning basic factual knowledge in medical education was often undervalued and could be enhanced by retrieval strategies.(34)

Insights from neuroscience

Retrieval involves accessing information in long-term memory, but much is still unknown about the neuroscientific basis of retrieval’s impact on strengthening memory. Scientists categorize the phases of memory processing into encoding, storage, and retrieval. How well information is retrieved depends upon encoding at the time of learning and cues at the time of retrieval.(33) It has been suggested that the process of retrieval reawakens stored memory traces, and recent animal studies have shown that the relationship between the neural pathway and retrieval cues for knowledge accessibility is important for retrieval success.(35)

There is significant evidence to support the theory of consolidation, a process by which newly learned material or experiences are transformed into long-term memory.(1) Consolidation involves both the hippocampus and the neocortex of the brain and occurs through the process of strengthening the neural connections.(1) These neural pathways are thought to be restructured by the repeated retrieval of the same information, which may result in changes in the synapses between neurons, in neuronal protein synthesis, and perhaps in the membrane potential of neurons.(36) It has also been hypothesized that the act of retrieval accelerates consolidation by supporting adaptive neural connections between the hippocampus and the neocortex.(37) The hippocampus, which has been described as an index for information storage in long-term memory, is thought to be involved in the retrieval process.(38) Other research has proposed a link between the consolidation process and the effects of retrieval-mediated learning.(39)

Practical Applications for Retrieval-Based Learning Strategies in Medical Education

Implementation in the classroom

One of the most significant challenges for medical educators is conveying to students the inadequacies of passive studying techniques, which remain quite popular among students, and convincing them of these. Examples of passive studying are reading over notes, rereading textbooks, highlighting large chunks of information, and ineffective use of flashcards in self-quizzing. There is still strong sentiment among many students that study techniques that make them feel good in the short term are preferable to active study strategies like retrieval, which they experience as difficult and frustrating.(17,40) It can be frustrating for educators to be met with student resistance to retrieval-based strategies that have been proven to be more effective and time-saving. Why do students continue to choose ineffective and inefficient study tactics? It has been suggested that it is because students generally determine their own learning strategies and choose methods that are quick and easy, giving them an illusion of knowing.(17) Students then do not further review the topics they have covered, and this is suboptimal for long-term knowledge retention.(12) A study that asked students to rank their study strategies found that the vast majority of students surveyed chose ineffective study methods (see the figure below).(17) When educators instruct their students about the benefits of retrieval strategies, there is some evidence that this will lead to greater utilization of these superior study tools.(41)

Student Study Strategy Usage. Rereading was found to be the most frequently used strategy by students despite the fact that it is less beneficial than other, more effective study strategies.(17)

Student study strategy

When focusing on classroom implementation, educators should take students’ sense of well-being into account. It is known that test anxiety can reduce the benefits of the testing effect.(42) In a study in which quizzes were given under differing conditions of high and low stress, both groups of students did equally well on the quizzes, but only the students who took the quizzes under low-stress conditions earned better scores on the subsequent criterion test.(42) Other research supports this assertion, indicating that educators may want to utilize low-stakes assessments to boost the power of retrieval strategies.(43,44) Perhaps if both teachers and students were aware of these findings, different choices would be made for more effective study strategies. The practice of retrieval may also protect against student stress. Researchers acknowledge that, although acute stress can impair memory, using practice tests could strengthen memory without the stress effects of taking a real test.(44)

So what are options for the implementation of retrieval in the classroom? Educators should identify the most important topics in their courses, teach them in their entirety, and then use short-answer questions with correct feedback in order to solidify knowledge.(21) Assigning ungraded quizzes or practice tests is a way to achieve the goal of minimizing the negative effects of test anxiety while promoting the benefits of the testing effect. Practicing retrieval can be as simple as asking students to put away materials after a lesson and then attempting to summarize the information presented that day or on previous days. These strategies have been referred to as “jots”, as in jot recall to a guiding question, jot sketches of anatomical relationships, or jot summaries of previously learned content.(1) Educators can also suggest the use of the Leitner system of flashcards, which employs the use of spaced retrieval: Students categorize information that they are more familiar with and retrieve those concepts at increasing intervals using the flashcards.(45) Instructors can give students practice quizzes or open-ended questions or even assign a concept map as a quiz on interrelated topics (which is a good way to introduce interleaving).(46) A think–pair–share strategy, in which the instructor poses a probing question to the class and students work first in partners and then report the information, is an excellent way to practice retrieval while also engaging in collaborative learning.(47)

Implementation in e-learning

Many experts advocate the utilization of technology to enhance retrieval-based learning strategies. For example, learning expert Daisy Christodoulou strongly recommends personalized digital learning platforms as the next best thing to cloning an instructor to meet each individual student’s educational needs.(48) Algorithms can take the guesswork out of what questions to ask of learners and when to ask them. They also provide unbiased feedback on answers to questions, saving teachers time and circumventing the problem of the illusion of knowing when students self-assess their knowledge.(17)

Some digital education platforms can keep track of questions correctly answered by students and rotate them through a spaced learning schedule. These digital learning platforms can be individualized by the learner to incorporate other techniques, such as interleaving, elaboration, concrete examples, and dual coding, all of which can enhance durable learning. E-learning platforms use various functionalities to implement spaced retrieval. Some employ tools that use a retrieval algorithm that returns questions to the learner based on specific characteristics, such as student confidence and correctness of responses. Originating from linguistic and mathematics learning, algorithms such as SuperMemo 2.0 have been used to automate spaced retrieval for some time now.(49) Some platforms utilize self-generated or community-generated questions and are then coupled with a similar retrieval algorithm, whereas other platforms, generally open-source and free, function more like an electronic flashcard system in which the user is responsible for establishing the spacing pattern.

The diagram below depicts a proposed visualization of the integration of retrieval-based strategies from the teacher’s and student’s perspectives. Students need to have clear learning objectives and criteria for evaluating their performance.(50) Educators should steer students toward evidence-based learning strategies (e.g., dual coding, elaboration, and concrete examples) and give timely feedback and guidance, even when a digital learning platform is utilized.(51) Students need to be assisted with appropriate goal setting, proper study materials, and retrieval opportunities, such a practice quizzes or low-stakes assessments targeted to the appropriate level of student knowledge.(52)

Schematic for the implementation of retrieval strategies (from Lecturio)

Spaced retrieval

Recommendations

Educator’s perspective

  • Implement retrieval-based strategies such as spaced retrieval and active recall. 
  • Utilize frequent low-stakes tests. These can be formative or summative depending on the makeup of your student body and educational goals. 
  • Educate students about the evidence that retrieval strategies, especially spaced retrieval, are superior and that it is metacognitive biases that keep students relying on inefficient study strategies.
  • Help students understand that retrieval practice is significantly more effective when it is spaced out rather than massed.
  • Make use of digital learning platforms and sophisticated algorithms to deliver effective retrieval methods of study that can customize learning tools for students.

Student’s perspective

  • If the course instructor does not offer digital learning platforms as a resource for the course, students can consider independent use of these resources to more easily implement spaced retrieval study methods.
  • Students lacking access to a digital platform can utilize flashcards according to the Leitner system, in which concepts that are known are sequenced through the deck less frequently than ones that are still being learned.
  • Students should recognize that effortful study strategies like retrieval are underutilized and that more popular studying tactics like rereading and highlighting are passive and therefore limited in facilitating long-term knowledge acquisition. 
  • Students should understand that when using retrieval strategies and self-checking their knowledge on retrieval tasks they may not have an impartial judgment on correctness. This could result in an illusion of knowing, negatively affecting their long-term performance.

Conclusion

Retrieval-based strategies such as spaced retrieval, active recall, and the testing effect are exceptionally effective learning tools. Their use does more than just better prepare students for assessments; it actually improves long-term retention of knowledge, which is critical for medical learners. Retrieval, specifically spaced retrieval, limits the effects of forgetting and facilitates the recall of information. Its relative ease of implementation in the classroom and the advantages of e-learning technologies that incorporate retrieval make retrieval-based strategies an essential element in medical teaching.

Self-Quiz

(Please select all that apply)

1. Which of the following learning strategies has been shown to enhance knowledge retention?

a. Spaced retrieval

b. Active recall

c. The testing effect 

d. All of the above

2. Which of the following statements are true concerning spaced retrieval?

a. It incorporates some of the spaced practice concepts.

b. Its implementation within an e-learning environment is enhanced by algorithms.

c. It is the only retrieval method that enhances memory.

d. There is significant research to support its utilization in classrooms.

3. The metacognitive practice of bringing to mind content can improve knowledge acquisition.

a. True

b. False

4. Research shows that students tend to choose study methods that are evidence-based and efficient for improving long-term retention.

a. True

b. False

5. Which of the following statements is supported by the cognitive science and neuroscientific evidence concerning retrieval-based strategies?

a. The process of retrieval is thought to enhance stored memory traces.

b. The illusion of knowing is not a factor involved in any type of retrieval practice.

c. The process of feedback should be delayed for some time after students practice retrieval, as it is part of desirable difficulties for students.

d. The process of retrieval is thought to accelerate the consolidation process.

Correct answers: (1) d. (2) a, b, d. (3) a. (4) b. (5) a, d.

Online Seminar

This online seminar from Durable Learning series will focus on the evidence-based method of retrieval practice to help conquer the “forgetting curve” and foster durable learning in medical students. We will examine the science behind the educational practices of spaced retrieval, active recall, and the testing effect, and suggest applications for your physical and digital classrooms.

Watch the seminar recording:

Authors

References

  1. Oakley B, EdD BR, Sejnowski TJ. Uncommon Sense Teaching: Practical Insights in Brain Science to Help Students Learn. New York: TarcherPerigee; 2021. 336 p.
  2. Ebbinghaus H. Memory; a contribution to experimental psychology [Internet]. New York city, Teachers college, Columbia university; 1913 [cited 2021 Sep 9]. 146 p. Available from: http://archive.org/details/memorycontributi00ebbiuoft
  3. American Physiological Association. APA Dictionary of Psychology [Internet]. 2020 [cited 2021 Nov 4]. Available from: https://dictionary.apa.org/
  4. Ferreira CS, Charest I, Wimber M. Retrieval aids the creation of a generalised memory trace and strengthens episode-unique information. NeuroImage. 2019 Nov;201:115996.
  5. Karpicke, Jeffrey. A powerful way to improve learning and memory [Internet]. https://www.apa.org. 2016 [cited 2021 Oct 26]. Available from: https://www.apa.org/science/about/psa/2016/06/learning-memory
  6. Bjork RA, Allen TW. The spacing effect: Consolidation or differential encoding? J Verbal Learn Verbal Behav. 1970 Oct 1;9(5):567–72.
  7. Locke EA. Toward a theory of task motivation and incentives. Organ Behav Hum Perform. 1968 May;3(2):157–89.
  8. Brown PC. Make it stick: the science of successful learning. Cambridge, Massachusetts: The Belknap Press of Harvard University Press; 2014. 313 p.
  9. Foy RL, Sya’ban SN, Wazir A, Horneffer PJ. How to Apply Spaced Practice to Make Learning in Medicine More Durable [Internet]. Lecturio – Learning Science. Available from: /pulse/how-to-apply-spaced-practice-to-make-learning-in-medicine-more-durable/
  10. Wazir A, Sya’ban SN, Horneffer PJ. Interleaving: How to Mix Related Concepts to Make Learning in Medicine More Durable [Internet]. Lecturio – Learning Science. [cited 2021 Nov 3]. Available from: /pulse/interleaving-how-to-mix-related-concepts-to-make-learning-in-medicine-more-durable/
  11. Cepeda NJ, Pashler H, Vul E, Wixted JT, Rohrer D. Distributed practice in verbal recall tasks: A review and quantitative synthesis. Psychol Bull. 2006;132(3):354–80.
  12. Agarwal PK, Bain PM. Powerful Teaching: Unleash the Science of Learning [Internet]. 1st ed. Wiley; 2019 [cited 2021 Apr 6]. Available from: https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/book/10.1002/9781119549031
  13. Carolina E. MW, Küpper-Tetzel, Weston T, Kim ASN, Kapler IV, Foot-Seymour V. Enhancing the Quality of Student Learning Using Distributed Practice. In: Dunlosky J, Rawson KA, editors. The Cambridge Handbook of Cognition and Education [Internet]. 1st ed. Cambridge University Press; 2019 [cited 2021 Nov 6]. p. 550–84. Available from: https://www.cambridge.org/core/product/identifier/9781108235631%23CN-bp-22/type/book_part
  14. Weinstein Y, Madan CR, Sumeracki MA. Teaching the science of learning. Cogn Res Princ Implic. 2018 Dec;3(1):2.
  15. Hultberg P, Santandreu Calonge D, Safiullin Lee AE. Promoting Long-lasting Learning Through Instructional Design. J Scholarsh Teach Learn [Internet]. 2018 Oct 2 [cited 2021 Nov 4];18(3). Available from: https://scholarworks.iu.edu/journals/index.php/josotl/article/view/23179
  16. The Learning Scientists- How Students Can Use Spacing and Retrieval Practice [Internet]. The Learning Scientists. [cited 2021 Nov 4]. Available from: https://www.learningscientists.org/learning-scientists-podcast/2018/3/7/episode-14-how-students-can-use-spacing-and-retrieval-practice
  17. Karpicke JD, Butler AC, Roediger HL. Metacognitive strategies in student learning: do students practise retrieval when they study on their own? Mem Hove Engl. 2009 May;17(4):471–9.
  18. Karpicke JD, Roediger HL. Expanding retrieval practice promotes short-term retention, but equally spaced retrieval enhances long-term retention. J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn. 2007;33(4):704–19.
  19. Kang SHK, Lindsey RV, Mozer MC, Pashler H. Retrieval practice over the long term: Should spacing be expanding or equal-interval? Psychon Bull Rev. 2014 Dec 1;21(6):1544–50.
  20. Rowland CA. The effect of testing versus restudy on retention: A meta-analytic review of the testing effect. Psychol Bull. 2014;140(6):1432–63.
  21. Greving S, Richter T. Examining the Testing Effect in University Teaching: Retrievability and Question Format Matter. Front Psychol. 2018;9:2412.
  22. Bjork RA, Bjork EL. Desirable Difficulties in Theory and Practice. J Appl Res Mem Cogn. 2020 Dec;9(4):475–9.
  23. Karpicke JD, Bauernschmidt A. Spaced retrieval: Absolute spacing enhances learning regardless of relative spacing. J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn. 2011;37(5):1250–7.
  24. Karpicke JD, Blunt JR. Retrieval Practice Produces More Learning than Elaborative Studying with Concept Mapping. Science. 2011 Feb 11;331(6018):772–5.
  25. Smith MA, Roediger HL, Karpicke JD. Covert retrieval practice benefits retention as much as overt retrieval practice. J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn. 2013;39(6):1712–25.
  26. Dunlosky J, Rawson KA, Marsh EJ, Nathan MJ, Willingham DT. Improving Students’ Learning With Effective Learning Techniques: Promising Directions From Cognitive and Educational Psychology. Psychol Sci Public Interest. 2013 Jan;14(1):4–58.
  27. Adesope OO, Trevisan DA, Sundararajan N. Rethinking the Use of Tests: A Meta-Analysis of Practice Testing. Rev Educ Res. 2017 Jun;87(3):659–701.
  28. Dobson JL. Retrieval practice is an efficient method of enhancing the retention of anatomy and physiology information. Adv Physiol Educ. 2013 Jun;37(2):184–91.
  29. Kromann CB, Jensen ML, Ringsted C. The effect of testing on skills learning. Med Educ. 2009;43(1):21–7.
  30. Kerfoot BP, DeWolf WC, Masser BA, Church PA, Federman DD. Spaced education improves the retention of clinical knowledge by medical students: a randomised controlled trial. Med Educ. 2007 Jan;41(1):23–31.
  31. Deng F, Gluckstein JA, Larsen DP. Student-directed retrieval practice is a predictor of medical licensing examination performance. Perspect Med Educ. 2015 Dec;4(6):308–13.
  32. Roediger III HL, Putnam AL, Smith MA. Ten Benefits of Testing and Their Applications to Educational Practice. In: Psychology of Learning and Motivation [Internet]. Elsevier; 2011 [cited 2021 Feb 16]. p. 1–36. Available from: https://linkinghub.elsevier.com/retrieve/pii/B9780123876911000016
  33. Kihlstrom, John. Principles of Learning and Memory [Internet]. Principles of Learning and Memory. 2015 [cited 2021 Oct 27]. Available from: https://www.ocf.berkeley.edu/~jfkihlstrom/MemoryWeb/index.htm
  34. Yeh DD, Park YS. Improving Learning Efficiency of Factual Knowledge in Medical Education. J Surg Educ. 2015 Sep;72(5):882–9.
  35. Frankland PW, Josselyn SA, Köhler S. The neurobiological foundation of memory retrieval. Nat Neurosci. 2019 Oct;22(10):1576–85.
  36. Dudai Y. The neurobiology of consolidations, or, how stable is the engram? Annu Rev Psychol. 2004;55:51–86.
  37. Antony JW, Ferreira CS, Norman KA, Wimber M. Retrieval as a Fast Route to Memory Consolidation. Trends Cogn Sci. 2017 Aug;21(8):573–6.
  38. Wiltgen BJ, Zhou M, Cai Y, Balaji J, Karlsson MG, Parivash SN, et al. The Hippocampus Plays a Selective Role in the Retrieval of Detailed Contextual Memories. Curr Biol. 2010 Aug;20(15):1336–44.
  39. Staresina BP, Cooper E, Henson RN. Reversible Information Flow across the Medial Temporal Lobe: The Hippocampus Links Cortical Modules during Memory Retrieval. J Neurosci. 2013 Aug 28;33(35):14184–92.
  40. Yeh DD, Park YS. Improving Learning Efficiency of Factual Knowledge in Medical Education. J Surg Educ. 2015 Sep;72(5):882–9.
  41. Einstein GO, Mullet HG, Harrison TL. The Testing Effect: Illustrating a Fundamental Concept and Changing Study Strategies. Teach Psychol. 2012 Jul;39(3):190–3.
  42. Hinze SR, Rapp DN. Retrieval (Sometimes) Enhances Learning: Performance Pressure Reduces the Benefits of Retrieval Practice. Appl Cogn Psychol. 2014;28(4):597–606.
  43. Khanna MM. Ungraded Pop Quizzes: Test-Enhanced Learning Without All the Anxiety. Teach Psychol. 2015 Apr 1;42(2):174–8.
  44. Smith AM, Floerke VA, Thomas AK. Retrieval practice protects memory against acute stress. Science. 2016 Nov 25;354(6315):1046–8.
  45. The Leitner System: How Does it Work? [Internet]. MindEdge. 2021 [cited 2021 Nov 3]. Available from: https://www.mindedge.com/learning-science/the-leitner-system-how-does-it-work/
  46. Alexaner M. Using Concept Maps in Assessment [Internet]. MedEd Matters. 2014 [cited 2021 Nov 4]. Available from: https://medicine.iu.edu/blogs/meded-matters/using-concept-maps-in-assessment
  47. Lightner J, Tomaswick L. Active Learning – Think, Pair, Share. [Internet]. Kent State University Center for Teaching and Learning. 2017 [cited 2021 Nov 3]. Available from: https://www.kent.edu/ctl/think-pair-share
  48. Christodoulou D. Teachers vs tech?: the case for an ed tech revolution. 2020.
  49. What is the SuperMemo spaced repetition method? [Internet]. [cited 2021 Nov 8]. Available from: https://www.supermemo.com/zh/about-supermemo-method#section-5
  50. Chatterjee D, Corral J. How to Write Well-Defined Learning Objectives. J Educ Perioper Med JEPM. 2017 Dec;19(4):E610.
  51. Crommelinck M, Anseel F. Understanding and encouraging feedback-seeking behaviour: a literature review: Feedback-seeking behaviour: a review. Med Educ. 2013 Mar;47(3):232–41.
  52. Pashler H, McDaniel M, Rohrer D, Bjork R. Learning Styles: Concepts and Evidence. Psychol Sci Public Interest. 2008 Dec;9(3):105–19.

Next Online Event

Never Miss a Pulse!

Get our insights direct to your inbox

Sign up for emails on new Lecturio Pulse content.

Sign up for emails on
new Lecturio Pulse content.

Never miss a pulse. We’ll email you when new content is published.

User Reviews