00:00
Okay, now when we do a randomized controlled
trial something often happens, that is, people
might change from one group to another, you
can probably imagine a scenario where that
might be the case. So I've allocated my subjects
to either the treatment group or the control
group. Something might arise, maybe someone
is responding in a different way, maybe I
misjudged their therapeutic state or something
like this and I have to change which group
they're in. It doesn't happen a lot, it does
happen occasionally. Now when they change
group, what do I do about it, I have some
options; I can either remove that person from
my analysis entirely or I could pretend they
were always in the group they ended up in
or I could pretend they're still in the group
they started in, which is better? Well if
we eliminate the individuals who move, what
happens is we introduce a kind of bias here,
because why did they move? Maybe they moved
because they're responding differently, a
number of scenarios comes to mind. So we don't
want to introduce any kind of bias, we like
to keep our subjects in the study as much
as we can, so that's not an option. If we
analyze them according to the treatment that
they received, for example, maybe they started
out in the control group but they moved to
the treatment group and now I'm going to analyze
them with everyone else who was in the treatment
group. Well that biases my analysis in favor
of finding a difference between the two groups.
We call this kind of analysis an on-protocol
or per-protocol analysis. Now again that kind
of analysis gives me a bias away from the
null hypothesis, that biases me towards rejecting
my null hypothesis. On the other hand, if
I analyze them according to the group they
started out in, even if they are getting the
new treatment, I'm pretending they're getting
the original treatment, we call that an intention
to treat analysis. It biases my conclusions
toward the null hypothesis, we prefer this.
01:59
Why do we prefer this, because science
is conservative, we'd like to make it as difficult
as possible to reject the null hypothesis,
such that when we do reject it, we can be
relatively satisfied it was done rationally
and because an effect is real. In other words,
we always do the intent to treat analysis
when we can. It's simply, logically and philosophically
preferred. One way to memorize this, is to
have the mnemonic; "If you randomize, you
analyze", once I've randomized people into
their groups, that's where they remain for
analysis purposes, even if they move after
that. If you randomize, analyze.
02:38
Let's talk about control groups now. A control
group is one of the most important foundational
ideas in science. If you think about it, anytime
I have a treatment group that is undergoing
some kind of therapy or treatment, I'm probably
always going to measure some kind of effect.
02:53
I don't know if that effect is large or small,
unless I'm comparing it to somebody, that's
why I need a control group, especially a control
group that has circumstances and environments
that are very, very similar, if not identical
to my treatment group with the exception of
the thing that I'm testing, maybe it's a drug.
So a control group is the most effective strategy,
well one of the most effective strategies,
for ruling out the effects of extraneous variables.
03:18
Why? Because I've controlled, hence the name
controlled, for all of the factors in both
groups, except for that one factor. When I
create a control group, I have some options
available to me. The control group can receive
either none of the treatment, this one's getting
a treatment, maybe a drug, maybe something
else, I can say my control gets nothing, or
I can say it gets the next logical standard
treatment, or it gets a placebo, which is
a kind of fake treatment. Let's think about
the next logical standard treatment for a
second, why does that matter? Well if I'm
testing a new drug, presumably it's to determine
if that drug is better than the one that is
currently on the market, not whether that
new drug is better than nothing. Pretty much
any new drug that I create after millions
of dollars of research is going to be better
than nothing. It only has value to me if I
can show that is better than what else I have.
So my control good here should be the other
drug that I have, the one that I'm currently
using because I'm trying to see if my new
drug is better than that. The other reason
we'd like to have the other current treatment
as a control group is for ethical reasons.
So imagine I'm testing a drug that's life-saving
or really important for pain management for
people who are very seriously ill, if I have
a control group made up of very sick people,
it's not ethical to give them nothing, I have
to give them something. The most ethical thing
to give them is the current recommended treatment.
04:48
It makes perfect sense and my study group
is getting this new treatment which I hope
is better. Placebo is another story entirely
and we'll talk about that in a second. Now
to draw appropriate valid comparisons between
a treatment group and a control group, I
have to assume that my groups are relatively
equivalent, equivalent in many ways, equivalent
in the distribution of variables and equivalent
in size. We call the equivalence in size, balance.
05:16
Balance is sometimes hard to achieve,
randomization techniques, advance randomization
techniques allow us to maximize the chance
that we'd have better balance between the
groups. When we have control groups though,
we have the possibility of biases. Now some
common biases in randomized controlled trials
are the Hawthorne effect and the Rosenthal
effect. We talked about these briefly in the
lecture on biases, but I want to go over them
again. Hawthorne effect is sometimes called
the observer effect. And it's when by the act
of simply observing someone, you change your
behavior. How this manifests in an RCT? Is
that subjects who are in a study modify their
behavior because they know they're in a study,
they know they are being watched by an investigator.
Maybe they want to please their investigator
and so they are behaving better. The Rosenthal
effect on the other hand is sometimes called
the Pygmalion effect and it's kind of the
opposite, it's when the investigator causes
a change in the subject simply by interacting
with them. Sometimes it's called a self-fulfilling
prophecy bias, because they're interacting
with their subject in such a way that they
want to elicit a change subconsciously and
they manage to do so. They do so by giving
subtle cues either verbally or physically.
So imagine we have in RCT that's trying to
measure whether or not a new drug reduces
the pain of migraine headaches, so we have
two groups, we have a treatment group getting
this new drug and we have a control group
that is not getting the drug, maybe they're
getting a placebo, maybe they're getting some
other drug. Now subjects may report feeling
less pain simply because they're in a study.
07:00
They're spending time with clinicians. They
are being looked at. Pain is one of those
classic cases of things that are subject to
this kind of bias, because it's so amorphous,
it's so difficult to measure objectively,
it's quite subjective. So maybe they are assuming
that they're getting the drug and not the
other thing that the control group is getting
and so they are feeling, "Ah, I must be getting
better", that's an example of Hawthorne effect.
07:23
When they are performing differently and better
simply because they think they're being watched
and spending time with the clinicians. Rosenthal
effect works in a different way, investigators
are eager to see their drug work. We spent
all this time and energy and money designing
an RCT to test this new drug because we expect
it to work. So maybe as a result,
as the investigator, I'm more enthusiastic
about the people who are getting the drug,
because "You're feeling better right? You're
feeling better. I want you to feel better."
Maybe I'm less enthusiastic about those who
are not getting the drug because I don't care
as much about them, let's say in theory, obviously
I do care about them. But they are not getting
the drug that I'm trying to test, so I'm spending
less time and less enthusiastic about them.
08:07
As a result, maybe the people who are receiving
the drug will respond differently, not because
of the drug, but because of me spending time
with them.